Jump to content

Stereoblindness

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Stereoblindness (also spelled stereo blindness) is the inability to perceive in three-dimensional (3D) depth using stereopsis, or stereo vision, by combining and comparing images from the two eyes.

Individuals with only one functioning eye have this condition by definition, as there is no visual input from the second eye. The condition can also occur when both eyes are healthy but do not function together properly.

Most stereoblind individuals with two healthy eyes use binocular vision to some extent, although less effectively than individuals with normal visual development. This was shown in a study in which stereoblind subjects were posed with the task of judging the direction of rotation of a simulated transparent cylinder: the subjects performed better when using two eyes than when using their preferred eye. They appeared to judge the direction of rotation using the images from each eye separately, then combined the judgments, rather than relying on differences between the images in both eyes.[1] Also, purely binocular motion stimuli appear to influence stereoblind persons' sensation of self-motion.[2] Furthermore, in some cases, each eye may contribute to peripheral vision on one side of the field of view (see also monofixation syndrome).

However, there is an exception: individuals with true congenital alternating squints have two healthy eyes and the ability to voluntarily switch between using either eye. However, stereoscopic and three-dimensional vision cannot be achieved in this condition. Attempts to train individuals with congenital alternating squints to use binocular vision often result in double vision, which may be irreversible.[citation needed]

Notable cases

[edit]

It has been suggested that the Dutch Old Master Rembrandt may have been stereoblind, which could have aided him in flattening visual perception for the creation of two-dimensional (2D) works.[3][4] Scientists have suggested that more artists seem to have stereoblindness when compared with a sample of people with stereo-acuteness (normal stereo vision).[5]

British neurologist Oliver Sacks lost his stereoscopic vision in 2009 due to a malignant tumor in his right eye, leaving him with no vision in that eye.[6] His loss of stereovision was recounted in his book The Mind's Eye, published in October 2010.[7]

In 2012, one case of stereoblindness was improved by watching a 3D film.[8]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Christa M. van Mierlo; Eli Brenner; Jeroen B.J. Smeets (2011). "Better performance with two eyes than with one in stereo-blind subjects' judgments of motion in depth". Vision Research. 51 (11): 1249–1253. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.015. PMID 21458479. S2CID 615787.
  2. ^ Jeremy M. Wolfe; Richard Held (March 1980). "Cyclopean stimulation can influence sensations of self-motion in normal and stereoblind subjects". Perception & Psychophysics. 28 (2): 139–142. doi:10.3758/bf03204339. PMID 7432987.
  3. ^ Marmor M. F., Shaikh S., Livingstone M. S., Conway B. R., Livingstone MS, Conway BR (September 2004). "Was Rembrandt stereoblind?". N. Engl. J. Med. 351 (12): 1264–5. doi:10.1056/NEJM200409163511224. PMC 2634283. PMID 15371590.
  4. ^ Rembrandt (van Rijn)
  5. ^ New York Times: A defect that may lead to a masterpiece (June 13, 2011)
  6. ^ "The Man Who Forgot How to Read and Other Stories", BBC accessed 30 June 2011
  7. ^ Murphy, John. "Eye to Eye with Dr. Oliver Sacks" Archived 2013-04-19 at the Wayback Machine, Review of Optometry, 9 December 2010
  8. ^ Peck, Morgen (2012-07-19). "How a movie changed one man's vision forever". BBC News. Retrieved July 20, 2012.

Bibliography

[edit]
[edit]